{"id":725,"date":"2011-07-02T17:11:11","date_gmt":"2011-07-02T15:11:11","guid":{"rendered":"http:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/?p=725"},"modified":"2023-07-24T12:19:15","modified_gmt":"2023-07-24T10:19:15","slug":"snipe-hunters-of-preys-with-low-epistemic-vigilance","status":"publish","type":"post","link":"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/blogs\/radu-umbres\/snipe-hunters-of-preys-with-low-epistemic-vigilance\/","title":{"rendered":"Snipe hunters of preys with low epistemic vigilance"},"content":{"rendered":"

What weapon would you use to hunt a dahu? Where would you start looking for a Volkswagen Beetle radiator hose? Does elbow grease come in cans or tubes? You shouldn\u2019t even begin thinking about these questions because they are just introductions into elaborate hoaxes. Dahus are fictional deers said to be adapted to the terrain of the Alps by having the feet on mountain side shorter than the feet on the valley side. The Beetle has an air-cooled engine and does not have a radiator. Elbow grease is a metaphor for strenuous manual labour. What makes these ficticious items successful cultural replicators? How can we explain the occurrence of \u201csnipe hunt\u201d in so many different social settings across the world?<\/p>\n

During my fieldwork in a Romanian village, I spent some time as an apprentice in a construction team.When one of our workmates accidentally broke a wooden plank, he was sent by the master builder to search for an \u201cacacia electrode\u201d to weld it back into one piece. One evening during after-work drinks, the team managed to convince a young villager that his skills as swimmer were needed early next morning, when the team was to build a dam on one of Romania\u2019s largest rivers. These practices are part of a cross-cultural set of practical jokes called the fool\u2019s errand or the snipe hunt.<\/p>\n

My favourite Wikipedia examples are:<\/p>\n

In construction, sending someone for a \u201cpipe-stretcher\u201d or \u201ccamouflage paint\u201d In offices, sending someone to buy \u201cverbal agreement forms\u201d In the navy, asking someone to get a visual on an incoming B1RD contact. In car repair, requesting someone to bring some diesel engine spark plugs In orchestras, requiring some members to find their \u201ctacet\u201d in their parts<\/p>\n

Another prank occurs in the Alps, Mexico, the Philippines and United States and many other places where na\u00efve individuals are coaxed into hunting non-existent animals with fantastical features. American Jackalopes are rabbits with antlers while their cousins, the Bavarian Wolpertingers, also have feathers and fangs. Folklorists have a long history of documenting the hunt for fantastical animals (see for instance here [1] and here [2]).<\/p>\n

I think that modern fool\u2019s errands and folkloric snipe hunts are basically the same prank which is based on an intentional exploitation of a recurrent failure of human communication. Let me unpack the prank into its components to demonstrate this. First of all, these pranks occur in a social interaction between non-equals. The party which is exploited is a na\u00efve participant. Na\u00efve not only in the sense that he (or she, but in the ethnographic examples I evoke, only men were involved) does not know that a practical joke is unfolding, but also na\u00efve in the sense of having little or no knowledge of the relevant domain. A fresh building apprentice does not know the names of most tools, a beginner sailor is unfamiliar with all the short codes used on deck and tourists are inexperienced in rural folk biology and taxonomy. The exploiter, on the other hand, is an expert in the domain: a master builder, a commanding officer or a local mountaineering guide. The success of the prank depends on the gullible party being convinced that the information transmitted is genuine and relevant. The fool must believe that the \u201cleft-handed screwdriver\u201d requested by the expert refers to a real artefact because the expert indicates his urgent need and uses technical jargon.<\/p>\n

If you are truly a novice, you do not know the object referred to as a \u201cleft-handed screwdriver\u201d, but nor do you most tools of the trade. Even if you do not a clear mental representation of the object, you defer to the master\u2019s authority, you believe \u201csemi-propositionally\u201d that he does need a left-handed screwdriver (whatever this may be) and, as a good apprentice, you go around looking for it. When you ask a second master for instructions, the goal of causing hilarity is achieved and, if the second master is also a skilled \u201csniper hunter\u201d, the joke may be kept alive by a new request.<\/p>\n

Why do we not only believe in this non-sense but also act upon such beliefs with most embarrassing consequences? The reason lies, I suggest, in the structure of the communicative act. The speaker is much more competent in the topic discussed than the target. The topic discussed is, moreover, filled with jargon and mental shortcuts. The jargon is almost never taught explicitly and participants learn many terms of the trade during practical tasks. [3] If the novice interprets the utterance as a genuine request, he also accepts the premise that the left-handed screwdriver refers to an actual object available nearby. When the novice accepts the assertion without further request for clarification, acts upon it and makes it known that he does so, he swallows the cognitive bait hook, line and sinker.<\/p>\n

Almost always, the artefact used in the prank seems genuine and needed but also surprising or even fantastical. The first requirement satisfies the condition of believability: you cannot trick someone with just any bizarre term. If I ask you to search for a bicycle carburettor, it might be too easy to for you to realise the absurdity of the request. On the other hand, the second requirement needs to be satisfied if the joke is to achieve its purpose. I can ask you to look for a (non-existent) green hammer, but that would hardly result in the outburst of laughter which accompanies a novice asking around for a glass hammer. The mastery of the prank is achieved by a maximisation of outlandishness within the limits of believability.<\/p>\n

The exploiters know, of course, that a beginner knows little but tries to be serviceable because of his subordinate position and desire to please. The joke is sometimes a collective enterprise in which the several \u201cinsiders\u201d play on the gullibility of a single(still) \u201coutsider\u201d and the greater the number of confederates or witnesses participating in the joke, the merrier it is.<\/p>\n

Here is a sketch of the causal mechanism which makes the \u201csnipe hunt\u201d a successful attempt to exploit an asymmetry of information and authority. The common features of cross-cultural practical jokes are:<\/p>\n

Actors:<\/p>\n

an initiator (or a group of them) who transmits an absurd request with the appearance of relevance; a victim whose epistemic vigilance<\/a> is at a low threshold due to the structure of unequal competence and social hierarchy;<\/p>\n

Scene:<\/p>\n

a conceptual domain characterized by a complex jargon and intensive communication relying on it. a structure of social interaction with a clearly defined, largely accepted difference of competence between experts and novices.<\/p>\n

This account goes somewhat towards an explanation of proximate causes, but leaves much to be explained for the persistence and reproduction of the \u201csnipe hunt\u201d. Why does it exist in so many cultures and technological domains? How is the prank reproduced\/changed over time? Is there any purpose for its use except good old fun? Let me venture a couple of hypothesis.<\/p>\n

The reproduction of the snipe hunt is partly based on the transformation of the novice into an expert through repeated pranks. After some time, the ex-victim becomes a victimiser for the current newcomers. He is now \u201cone of the boys\u201d and he can practice his skill of deceit which asserts his new status in the pecking order of the social group.<\/p>\n

The \u201csnipe hunt\u201d might play a role in the \u201ceducation of attention\u201d in the social group.Na\u00efve participants are taught by recourse to a cruel example the importance of discriminating between relevant and irrelevant information. Especially in the construction trade, much of the communication is non-verbal or non-directive and requires extensive self-reliance, simultaneous representation of a task, and participatory imagination. The fool\u2019s errand can, thus, be a lesson in how not be a fool i.e. to become a competent, useful member of the team\/group\/community. Supporting evidence could be the integration of the snipe hunt in the Boy Scout general training routine. [4]\n

I will end with a case of \u201csnipe hunting\u201d which takes the ethnographer as its target due to his ignorance of local beliefs and characters. In a builder\u2019s culture valuing manhood and physical endurance, I was the only member of the team using protection gear. One day, my master builder told me that he was quite upset; he had been approached by an old villager, Uncle Toma, who asked whether the beautiful houses were the design of that new fellow, the engineer with the bright orange hard hat spending his time on the site and sometimes working on his laptop in the village pub. The master was angry that his skill was not acknowledged and that my showing off with smart contraptions put me in an unearned limelight. Several other team members advised me to \u201ckeep it cool\u201d, and stop being such a poser. The entire story was an elaborate hoax; there was no old man (of course I asked around and made a fool of myself), no such gossip, everyone knew very well who was who and who did what in the building site. I was taken for a (cultural) ride as a na\u00efve ethnographer with a very sensitive awareness of his ambiguous role in the village.<\/p>\n

I have a few questions of my own for fellow C&C members. Have you been taken aboard for the snipe hunt in your fieldwork or personal life? Do you think there is a connection with initiation rituals such as hazing? What is the role played by laughter and humour in this prank? Can we discard some of the particular features of the snipe hunt in order to achieve a general model of recurrent elaborate deceit?<\/p>\n


\n[1] Smith, J. H. (1957). In the Bag: A Study of Snipe Hunting.\u00a0Western Folklore<\/i>,\u00a016<\/i>(2), 107-110.<\/p>\n[2] Chartois, J., & Claudel, C. (1945). Hunting the Dahut: a French folk custom.\u00a0The Journal of American Folklore<\/i>,\u00a058<\/i>(227), 21-24.
\n[3] In our building team, we collectively used the term \u201cpetalude\u201d (from Gr. Petaloudes) to refer to an implement resembling a butterfly because our master learned his trade and the terminology in Greece and always called for the implement by this name (in the beginning, pointing out the tool at the same time). No-one in the village would understand the meaning of this term and many others like it, something very useful in our secretive trade.<\/p>\n[4] Ellis, B. (1981). The camp mock-ordeal theater as life.\u00a0The Journal of American Folklore<\/i>,\u00a094<\/i>(374), 486-505.<\/p>\n","protected":false},"excerpt":{"rendered":"

What weapon would you use to hunt a dahu? Where would you start looking for a Volkswagen Beetle radiator hose? Does elbow grease come in cans or tubes? You shouldn\u2019t even begin thinking about these questions because they are just introductions into elaborate hoaxes. Dahus are fictional deers said to be adapted to the terrain […]<\/p>\n","protected":false},"author":699,"featured_media":0,"comment_status":"open","ping_status":"closed","sticky":false,"template":"","format":"standard","meta":{"footnotes":""},"categories":[20],"tags":[],"acf":[],"yoast_head":"\nSnipe hunters of preys with low epistemic vigilance - International Cognition and Culture Institute<\/title>\n<meta name=\"robots\" content=\"index, follow, max-snippet:-1, max-image-preview:large, max-video-preview:-1\" \/>\n<link rel=\"canonical\" href=\"http:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/blogs\/radu-umbres\/snipe-hunters-of-preys-with-low-epistemic-vigilance\/\" \/>\n<meta property=\"og:locale\" content=\"en_GB\" \/>\n<meta property=\"og:type\" content=\"article\" \/>\n<meta property=\"og:title\" content=\"Snipe hunters of preys with low epistemic vigilance - International Cognition and Culture Institute\" \/>\n<meta property=\"og:description\" content=\"What weapon would you use to hunt a dahu? Where would you start looking for a Volkswagen Beetle radiator hose? Does elbow grease come in cans or tubes? You shouldn\u2019t even begin thinking about these questions because they are just introductions into elaborate hoaxes. Dahus are fictional deers said to be adapted to the terrain […]\" \/>\n<meta property=\"og:url\" content=\"http:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/blogs\/radu-umbres\/snipe-hunters-of-preys-with-low-epistemic-vigilance\/\" \/>\n<meta property=\"og:site_name\" content=\"International Cognition and Culture Institute\" \/>\n<meta property=\"article:published_time\" content=\"2011-07-02T15:11:11+00:00\" \/>\n<meta property=\"article:modified_time\" content=\"2023-07-24T10:19:15+00:00\" \/>\n<meta name=\"twitter:card\" content=\"summary\" \/>\n<meta name=\"twitter:label1\" content=\"Written by\">\n\t<meta name=\"twitter:data1\" content=\"Radu Umbres\">\n\t<meta name=\"twitter:label2\" content=\"Estimated reading time\">\n\t<meta name=\"twitter:data2\" content=\"7 minutes\">\n<script type=\"application\/ld+json\" class=\"yoast-schema-graph\">{\"@context\":\"https:\/\/schema.org\",\"@graph\":[{\"@type\":\"WebSite\",\"@id\":\"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/#website\",\"url\":\"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/\",\"name\":\"International Cognition and Culture Institute\",\"description\":\"\",\"potentialAction\":[{\"@type\":\"SearchAction\",\"target\":\"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/?s={search_term_string}\",\"query-input\":\"required name=search_term_string\"}],\"inLanguage\":\"en-GB\"},{\"@type\":\"WebPage\",\"@id\":\"http:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/blogs\/radu-umbres\/snipe-hunters-of-preys-with-low-epistemic-vigilance\/#webpage\",\"url\":\"http:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/blogs\/radu-umbres\/snipe-hunters-of-preys-with-low-epistemic-vigilance\/\",\"name\":\"Snipe hunters of preys with low epistemic vigilance - International Cognition and Culture Institute\",\"isPartOf\":{\"@id\":\"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/#website\"},\"datePublished\":\"2011-07-02T15:11:11+00:00\",\"dateModified\":\"2023-07-24T10:19:15+00:00\",\"author\":{\"@id\":\"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/#\/schema\/person\/eb6665358141840dce481cc830e41032\"},\"inLanguage\":\"en-GB\",\"potentialAction\":[{\"@type\":\"ReadAction\",\"target\":[\"http:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/blogs\/radu-umbres\/snipe-hunters-of-preys-with-low-epistemic-vigilance\/\"]}]},{\"@type\":\"Person\",\"@id\":\"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/#\/schema\/person\/eb6665358141840dce481cc830e41032\",\"name\":\"Radu Umbres\",\"image\":{\"@type\":\"ImageObject\",\"@id\":\"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/#personlogo\",\"inLanguage\":\"en-GB\",\"url\":\"http:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/wp-content\/uploads\/avatar1487508664-96x96.png\",\"caption\":\"Radu Umbres\"},\"description\":\"Romanian cognitive anthropologist, fieldwork in a NE Romanian village. \\u00a0Interested in folk epistemology, trust, morality, communication. University Lecturer in Anthropology at the Faculty of Political Sciences, SNSPA, Bucharest\",\"sameAs\":[\"https:\/\/institutnicod.academia.edu\/RUmbres\"]}]}<\/script>\n<!-- \/ Yoast SEO plugin. -->","_links":{"self":[{"href":"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/wp-json\/wp\/v2\/posts\/725"}],"collection":[{"href":"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/wp-json\/wp\/v2\/posts"}],"about":[{"href":"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/wp-json\/wp\/v2\/types\/post"}],"author":[{"embeddable":true,"href":"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/wp-json\/wp\/v2\/users\/699"}],"replies":[{"embeddable":true,"href":"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/wp-json\/wp\/v2\/comments?post=725"}],"version-history":[{"count":3,"href":"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/wp-json\/wp\/v2\/posts\/725\/revisions"}],"predecessor-version":[{"id":17228,"href":"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/wp-json\/wp\/v2\/posts\/725\/revisions\/17228"}],"wp:attachment":[{"href":"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/wp-json\/wp\/v2\/media?parent=725"}],"wp:term":[{"taxonomy":"category","embeddable":true,"href":"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/wp-json\/wp\/v2\/categories?post=725"},{"taxonomy":"post_tag","embeddable":true,"href":"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/wp-json\/wp\/v2\/tags?post=725"}],"curies":[{"name":"wp","href":"https:\/\/api.w.org\/{rel}","templated":true}]}}