piece<\/a> in the New York Times “The Americanization of mental illness”, published on the 10th of january. The essay is adapted from Ethan Watters\u2019 forthcoming book, Crazy Like Us: The Globalization of the American Psyche.<\/p>\nAs with many fashionable ideas,\u00a0 it is a bit difficult to isolate the arguments from the seductive examples. The thesis itself, as it appears in the paper, leaves room to different interpretations: “Mental illnesses are not discrete entities like the polio virus with their own natural histories\u2026.and have never been the same the world over (either in prevalence or in form) but are inevitably sparked and shaped by the ethos of particular times and places.” What is sparked and shaped by culture? The boundary between mental illness and mental health, the distinction between mental and physical illness, or the division between kinds of mental illnesses? Some examples in the article even suggest that cultural classifications of mental illnesses converge, but give different explanations of their origins, significance or treatments. Others stress the fact that what is spreading is basically a “symptom repertoire”, i.e. knowledge of how to diagnose illnesses, rather than definitions of what is diagnosed.<\/p>\n
Moreover, as nobody challenges the idea that different cultures have different views on health and medicine, which in their turn influence the treatments people are \u2013 or are not \u2013 offered, the revolutionary potential of the thesis can be a bit hard to see.<\/p>\n
But the paper highlights two more interesting, or disturbing points:\u00a0first, that western categories of mental illnesses spread and contaminates the other cultures, and second, that this contagion is not for the best.<\/p>\n
In this sense, as I understand it, \u201cmind medicine\u201d would stand in contrast with \u201cbody medicine\u201d, either because the contagion isn\u2019t so unilateral (see the rise of Chinese medicine or acupuncture in Europe) or because nobody thinks that the spread of western ideas about cancer or AIDS isn\u2019t a good thing. Anyway, the paper presents a good case of contagion of ideas \u2013 and even puts it on trial. The two aspects are of interest, although a bit confused.<\/p>\n
The ways the world goes mad<\/p>\n
The paper relies heavily on a recent and documented example, showing how \u00ab anorexia \u00bb spread in Hong Kong after a tragic case where the press reported that a young, very thin girl had died of \u201canorexia\u201d.\u00a0 \u201cFood refusal had a particular expression and meaning in Hong Kong\u201d and\u00a0 \u201cunlike American anorexics, most of (the) patients did not intentionally diet nor did they express a fear of becoming fat\u201d. It would almost be a case of contagion from a single case: after the event caught the press attention, doctors saw more patients reporting or showing \u00ab western \u00bb symptoms of anorexia. “By 2007 about 90 percent of the anorexics \u2026treated reported fat phobia. New patients appeared to be increasingly conforming their experience of anorexia to the Western version of the disease.”<\/p>\n
Ironically, all the mental illnesses categories of anorexia, hysteria, schizophrenia are said to be “American” – perhaps the author signs a bit too quickly of the fact that ideas are shaped by one own\u2019s culture.<\/p>\n
The case is certainly very interesting, and well chosen. “Eating disorders” are multi-faceted, and their varieties, the real nature of the pathology, their mix with normative aspects of bodily appearance, their significance are still objects of inquiry. Still, the shift between the level of symptoms (food starvation), the mental attitudes of patients (fat phobia) doesn\u2019t help seeing whether :<\/p>\n
any person who starves herself (or himself) is an anorexic, \nany person who starves herself (or himself) has a form of anorexia (there is no anorexia, but just family resemblances between cultural expressions of anorexia) \nonly persons who starve themselves because of fat phobia are anorexic \npersons who starve themselves and are ready to believe that this is because of fat phobia become real anorexic \npersons who starve themselves and are ready to believe that this is because of fat phobia are “fake” anorexic.<\/p>\n
There is a “loop” : people who starve themselves come to believe they are anorexic and they become “real” anorexic.<\/p>\n
I guess one needs to be clear on what is at stake, before putting forward a thesis on the \u201cwesternization of mental illnesses\u201d.<\/p>\n
The spirits do better<\/p>\n
The paper goes on showing why this spreading of ideas is not for the best.\u00a0 A bit of “colonial bad conscience” pervades the whole paper, but doesn\u2019t go without arguments.<\/p>\n
Here, the case study is schizophernic (or schizophrenic-like) people \u2013a case which is quite different from anorexia, at least because its neurological aspects are now well established. Anyway, the focus here is on treatment.\u00a0 The trouble comes here from the fact that the World Health Organization established, after a research over the course of 30 years, that \u00ab\u00a0patients outside the United States and Europe had significantly lower relapse rates \u2014 as much as two-thirds lower in one follow-up study. These findings have been widely discussed and debated in part because of their obvious incongruity: the regions of the world with the most resources to devote to the illness \u2014 the best technology, the cutting-edge medicines and the best-financed academic and private-research institutions \u2014 had the most troubled and socially marginalized patients.\u201d<\/p>\n
Two explanations are offered:<\/p>\n
First, spirit possession is more “democratic”: anybody “violating social norms \u2014 from a sister lashing out at her brother to someone beset by psychotic delusions” is possessed, and thus possession doesn\u2019t result in stigmatisation and exclusion. (The Freudian idea that we are all perverse must thus be a effective one !). But here, the boundary between health and illness is cancelled, and it\u2019s not clear that it supports the idea that different conceptions of illness have different effects.<\/p>\n
Second, people would universally have tendency to “essentialize” the biological and thus, western biological theories of mental illness (reducing them to brain chemistry disorders) tends to make the “mad” essentially (and permanently) mad, while traditional conceptions of spirit possession makes him or her only contingently (and temporarily) mad. The latter encourage a better integration of possessed people, finally helping them to be better, and reducing the frequency or intensity of their crises.<\/p>\n
The paper reports experiments by Prof. Sheila Mehta, from Auburn University Montgomery in Alabama,\u00a0 that I hadn\u2019t heard of \u2013 and are fairly disturbing. The hypothesis was that “biomedical explanations for mental illness might be influencing our attitudes toward the mentally ill in ways we weren\u2019t conscious of.”<\/p>\n
“Test subjects were led to believe that they were participating in a simple learning task with a partner who was, unbeknownst to them, a confederate in the study. Before the experiment started, the partners exchanged some biographical data, and the confederate informed the test subject that he suffered from a mental illness. The confederate then stated either that the illness occurred because of \u201cthe kind of things that happened to me when I was a kid\u201d or that he had \u201ca disease just like any other, which affected my biochemistry.\u201d \u2026.The experiment then called for the test subject to teach the confederate a pattern of button presses. When the confederate pushed the wrong button, the only feedback the test subject could give was a \u201cbarely discernible\u201d to \u201csomewhat painful\u201d electrical shock\u201d. At the end \u201cThose who believed that their partner suffered a biochemical \u201cdisease like any other\u201d increased the severity of the shocks at a faster rate than those who believed they were paired with someone who had a mental disorder caused by an event in the past.\u201d<\/p>\n
Does this effect still applies to people coming from non-western cultures\u00a0? And don\u2019t these two explanations show that\u00a0 actually, some explanations (ie the spirit possession) are absolutely better than others, and not \u2013 as I believe the paper wants to show \u2013 locally and relatively better\u00a0(spirit possession is good-in-Zanzibar, and brain-explanations are good-in-America)\u00a0? Any thought ?<\/p>\n","protected":false},"excerpt":{"rendered":"
Relativity of mental illness has enjoyed the favours of philosophers for decades (Michel Foucault, Ian Hacking and, more recently Geoffrey Llyod in his Cognitive Variations). It has lead to the development of the “new cross-cultural psychiatry”, heralded by Kleinman in 1977. It\u00a0 may become the best pop version of culture and cognition \u2013 as shown […]<\/p>\n","protected":false},"author":910,"featured_media":0,"comment_status":"open","ping_status":"closed","sticky":false,"template":"","format":"standard","meta":{"footnotes":""},"categories":[28],"tags":[],"acf":[],"yoast_head":"\n
Mad in America - International Cognition and Culture Institute<\/title>\n \n \n \n \n \n \n \n \n \n \n \n \n\t \n\t \n\t \n