{"id":12589,"date":"2020-03-31T09:57:00","date_gmt":"2020-03-31T07:57:00","guid":{"rendered":"http:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/?p=12589"},"modified":"2023-08-09T14:15:42","modified_gmt":"2023-08-09T12:15:42","slug":"do-covid-19-conspiracy-theories-stem-from-gullibility-or-skepticism","status":"publish","type":"post","link":"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/blogs\/inge-van-de-ven\/do-covid-19-conspiracy-theories-stem-from-gullibility-or-skepticism\/","title":{"rendered":"Do COVID-19 conspiracy theories stem from gullibility or skepticism?"},"content":{"rendered":"\n
\"\"<\/a><\/figure>\n\n\n\n

When it comes to the COVID-19 pandemic that has the world in its grip, gaps in public knowledge still abound. It is therefore not surprising that during the last weeks a whole bunch of \u2018interesting\u2019 theories and speculations regarding \u2018what is really<\/em> going on\u2019 have surfaced. Did the virus originate with very hungry Chinese people eating bats (Baragona, Watson 2020), or has it been deliberately spread by world leaders to counteract the ageing of the population? Did the US create the virus to justify an economic war on China? Is it a plot to destabilize Iran? Far right Youtubers try to convince their enormous audience that \u2018globalism\u2019 and open borders are to be blamed for the rapid spread of the virus (Watson 2020a). Italy\u2019s rightwing populist leaders Salvini and Meloni are spreading debunked conspiracy theories through Facebook and Twitter: e.g. in 2015, a Chinese lab already created a \u2018Supercoronavirus\u2019 from bats and mice (Nardelli & D\u2019Urso, 2020). Influencers and Russian media helpfully contribute that the virus might originate from 5G wireless technology use. Even after such theories have been debunked by media outlets and fact checkers, they generate millions of views on social media. [1] They seem to be spreading almost as fast as the virus itself.<\/p>\n\n\n\n

Then there are the less serious, just mildly annoying forms of misinformation like the daily emails I receive from companies with ads masquerading as public service, public health information, or concern for my wellbeing: \u201cDon\u2019t worry, we will still cut your hair\u201d or \u201cour pizzas are now half off and we\u2019ll throw them at your door from a distance\u201d. <\/p>\n\n\n\n

Of course, fake news and conspiracies have been around since humankind. Spreading uncertain information or rumors could even be considered a social means of survival (Radnitz 2020). Our current online media only extend these existing practices\u2014in speed, accessibility, and scale. Yet in a pandemic, misinformation can have harmful effects and even cost lives as people base their actions (and non-actions) on it.<\/p>\n\n\n\n

We were, in fact, not<\/em> born yesterday!<\/strong> <\/p>\n\n\n\n

Are we then so ready to accept anything anyone tells us? In Not Born Yesterday: The Science of Who we Trust and Who we Believe<\/em> (2020), Hugo Mercier answers this question with a firm \u2018no\u2019. Humans, he claims, are not gullible; in fact, we are very good at deciding in whom we place our trust. Mercier presents a range of evidence from political science, history, and anthropology, stating that historical attempts at mass persuasion by demagogues and propagandists have in fact failed most of the time. He draws on experimental psychology to explain why this is: humans are equipped with sophisticated cognitive mechanisms of \u2018open vigilance\u2019. These allow us to filter the flow of information surrounding us. These mechanisms compute a range of cues to decide how much we should believe of what others tell us, and thus prompt vigilance against potentially damaging beliefs. We compare new information with what we already hold true, and in case it is incongruent, reject this information. At the same time, these mechanisms are \u2018open\u2019 enough to allow us to change our minds, if<\/em> we have reasons to believe the source of the information is well-informed, competent, or offers sound argumentation: in short, if we consider the source trustworthy<\/em>. Mercier optimistically chooses to interpret failures in this system (instances in which false information is accepted) as \u2018bugs\u2019 to overcome in otherwise effective cognitive mechanisms, instead of signs of gullibility.<\/p>\n\n\n\n

So if we aren\u2019t na\u00efve, then what exactly\nare we? Mercier goes with \u201crationally skeptical\u201d. In the absence of good\nreasons to change our minds, he says, we simply don\u2019t; yet people we trust can\npersuade us. Trust is therefore especially important in times of information\noverload: <\/p>\n\n\n\n

\n

Because we don\u2019t have the time, the motivation, or even the extra information required to properly evaluate most of the information we encounter, we revert to a state of rational skepticism. … the largest problem with the current informational environment isn\u2019t the information we accept which we should have rejected, but the information we reject which we should (had we had more information) have accepted. (Mercier 2020a)<\/p>\n<\/blockquote>\n\n\n\n

He calls this \u2018information asymmetry,\u2019 which means that we gather more knowledge and reap more beneficial effects from trusting than from not trusting. You get more information when you trust, including information on whether the source turns out to be right, hence trustworthy. However, if the stakes are very high\u2014say, in times of a global pandemic\u2014we are inclined to not trust enough, afraid of what it will cost us.<\/p>\n\n\n\n

Media literacy and a hermeneutic of suspicion<\/strong><\/p>\n\n\n\n

In highlighting the role of the media in all this, Piia Varis (2018) offers a valuable addition to Mercier\u2019s study. Varis considers the role of the Internet in how we determine what is true. The media are by no means neutral actors; they increase the visibility of certain content. Sensationalist messages perform especially well in attention-based digital media in terms of spreadability (Mihailidis & Viotty 2017). Thus, the algorithms of Youtube are known to aid the rapid spread of conspiracy theories, which show up as top search results. Platforms like Reddit, with its affordances such of up- and downvoting, offer new ways to establish expertise and authority, and facilitate the formation of new groups around topics of interest. Today, such technological and social affordances are game changers in knowledge production. <\/p>\n\n\n\n

Going back to Mercier\u2019s thesis, and taking this media sphere into account, I would agree that we are not gullible: I might even go one step further, and ask if in Western culture, media users have not become too skeptical for their own good. In Europe and the United States, trust in the media has been steadily eroding since the 1960s (Bialik and Matsa 2017). Americans increasingly suspect mainstream media of bias and are driven to find more objective news sources (Gallup and Knight 2018). Academics, traditional outlets, and bureaucratic news sources alike face an erosion of trust from the public. Lay people often parse information according to an interpretative strategy that we might call a \u2018hermeneutic of suspicion\u2019 (Ricoeur 1965). They are alert to being are gaslighted by mainstream news media or our governments. <\/p>\n\n\n\n

Danah boyd has made the observation that while media literacy is often hailed as the solution to all this, it can be fact be seen as the problem itself. That is, if we take media literacy to mean teaching students to \u2018do the research\u2019 for themselves, which often entails relying on Google to find answers to their questions. Self-investigation, and getting \u2018informed\u2019 as a personal responsibility, is not always a good thing when we do not all agree what constitutes a trusted source. In a climate of doubt and skepticism (Conway & Oreskes 2010), and in communities where \u2018the liberal media\u2019 are distrusted, urging young people to find out for themselves who is to be trusted can be sketchy, especially as radical web forums abound offering them \u2018the red pill,\u2019 or a more radical, alternative view of reality. They are primed to doubt information, question authority, and look for hidden agendas. <\/p>\n\n\n\n

In case of the current global pandemic, the invisibility of the virus as an object of knowledge, and the lack of data, make it impossible to determine in any positivistic manner \u2018who was right,\u2019 and it could take a while before we could determine in hindsight who was worthy of our trust. We could, with Golebiewski and boyd (2018) speak of a \u2018data void\u2019. Even scientists work with theories until these are falsified. When new facts become known, and they have to make new inferences, that doesn\u2019t mean they were spreading fake news. Updating models and hypotheses in light of new data is normal scientific practice. Conspiracy theorists, alt- and far right activists, political influencers, are eager to fill the knowledge gaps with more sweeping theories. They score very high on media literacy. They know how to optimize their content to come out on top of the Youtube searches (Maly 2018). <\/p>\n\n\n\n

Collective (un)intelligence: COVID as transmedia storytelling?<\/strong><\/p>\n\n\n\n

The current state of technology and the availability of new communal platforms, as well as an increasing level of media literacy, have undermined the monopoly of the old media institutions: a development heralded by Henry Jenkins in 2003 under the flag of \u2018convergence culture\u2019. Back then, Jenkins was optimistic: participating in collective processes of interpretation and communication, he thought, could foster democratic decision-making processes. Lay people with shared interests, or \u2018knowledge communities\u2019, come together to solve puzzles. Media users have become hunters and gatherers, piecing together narratives across different channels: this, Jenkins called transmedia storytelling. Collective intelligence is needed for virtual communities to operationalize the aggregate skills and knowledge of their individual members. In knowledge communities, reading practices and interpretative strategies include a meticulous, almost obsessive attention to detail and a readiness (and, these weeks, a lot of spare time) to engage in time-consuming and painstaking analyses and close readings\/viewings. Together, we see, hear, and understand more: collective intelligence can be seen as a contemporary spin on what Walter Benjamin (1999) called the deepening of apperception through technology.<\/p>\n\n\n\n

Jenkins of course writes of transmedia storytelling with regards to fictional universes like Lost or Twin Peaks, marked by a \u2018conspirational mode of storytelling\u2019 (Brinker 2012). Such series are like conspiracy theories, in that they show an underlying organizational logic of gaining progressive insight into hidden plans of some scheming party, while the number of unexplained mysteries and unanswered plot questions multiplies and more and more sinister plots come to the surface (Barkun 2003). Are such series so popular because this is how many people experience the real world? Are they priming us to become even more vigilant? In any case, the distinction between leisurely consumption of fictional media on the one hand, and knowledge production about real issues online on the other, becomes increasingly unclear. Distrust of official narratives marks a lot of what happens on discussion forums and social media online. Fact and fiction, truth and rumor, are increasingly blurred. <\/p>\n\n\n\n

What happens in a data void, is that we start filling in the gaps through speculation, gathering crumbs of information and piecing them together. COVID-19 becomes an event of transmedia storytelling, emblematic for our current media landscape. In conclusion, we are far from gullible. Practices of close reading and close viewing are alive and kicking. Many of us are media literate, as well as narratively literate. There is, however, a thin line between media literacy and paranoia.<\/p>\n\n\n


\n\n\n

References<\/strong><\/p>\n\n\n\n

Baragona, Justin. \u201cFox News Host Claims Chinese People Eating \u2018Raw Bats\u2019 to Blame for Coronavirus.<\/em>\u201d Daily Beast, 2 March.<\/a><\/p>\n\n\n\n

Barkun, Michael. A Culture of Conspiracy. Apocalyptic Visions in Contemporary America<\/em>. Berkeley: Unviersity of California Press, 2003.<\/p>\n\n\n\n

Bialik, K, and K.E. Matsa. (2017). \u201cKey trends in social and digital news media.\u201d Pew Research Center <\/em>(2017): 4-10.<\/p>\n\n\n\n

Benjamin, Walter. Benjamin, Walter. \u201cThe Work of Art in the Age of Mechanical Reproduction\u201d Illuminations<\/em>. Ed. Hannah Arendt. Trans. Harry Zorn. London: Pimlico, 1999 [1968]. 211-44.<\/p>\n\n\n\n

boyd, d. \u201cDid Media Literacy Backfire?\u201d Data and Society<\/em> (2017).<\/a><\/p>\n\n\n\n

boyd, d. \u201cYou Think You Want Media Literacy\u2026 Do You?\u201d Data and Society<\/em> (2018).<\/a><\/p>\n\n\n\n

Brinker, F. (2012). \u201cHidden Agendas, Endless Investigations, and the Dynamics of Complexity: The Conspiratorial Mode of Storytelling in Contemporary American Television Series.\u201d aspeers<\/em> 5: 87-109.<\/p>\n\n\n\n

Conway, E., and N. Oreskes. (2010). Merchants of Doubt. How a Handful of Scientists Obscured the Truth on Issues from Tobacco Smoke to Global Warming<\/em>. Bloomsbury.<\/p>\n\n\n\n

Gallup and Knight, \u201cAmerican Views: Trust, Media and Democracy,\u201d Knight Foundation Report <\/em>(2018).<\/a><\/p>\n\n\n\n

Golebiewski, M. and boyd, d. (2018). Data Voids: Where Missing Data Can Easily Be Exploited. Data & Society.<\/em><\/a><\/p>\n\n\n\n

Maly, I. (2019). New Right Metapolitics and the Algorithmic Activism of Schild & Vrienden. Social Media + Society<\/em>.<\/a><\/p>\n\n\n\n

Maly, I. (2020). \u201cThe coronavirus, the attention economy and far-right junk news.\u201d Diggit Magazine<\/em>.<\/a><\/p>\n\n\n\n

Mercier, H. (2020). Not Born Yesterday: The Science of Who we\nTrust and Who we Believe. <\/em>Princeton U.P.<\/p>\n\n\n\n

Mercier, H. (2020a). \u201cWhat do you really know about gullibility?\u201d Princeton University Press, website<\/em>, 08 Jan.<\/a><\/p>\n\n\n\n

Mihailidis, P. & Viotty, S. (2017). Spreadable\nSpectacle in Digital Culture: Civic Expression, Fake News, and the Role of\nMedia Literacies in \u201cPost-Fact\u201d Society. American Behavioral Scientist<\/em> 61(4).<\/p>\n\n\n\n

Nardelli, A. and D\u2019Urso, D. (2020). \u201cItaly\u2019s Far-Right And Nationalist Leaders Are Pushing Debunked Conspiracy Theories About The Coronavirus To Millions Of Followers\u201d. Buzzfeed News, 26 March.<\/a><\/p>\n\n\n\n

Radnitz, Scott. (2020). \u201cWhy conspiracy theories spread faster than coronavirus.\u201d  The Guardian<\/em>, 3 March.<\/a><\/p>\n\n\n\n

Ricoeur, P. (1970) Freud and Philosophy: An Essay on Interpretation<\/em>. Trans. D. Savage,\nYale U.P.<\/p>\n\n\n\n

Tibken, S. (2020). \u201cNo, 5G didn’t cause the coronavirus pandemic.\u201d Cnet<\/em>, 18 March.<\/a><\/p>\n\n\n\n

Varis, P. (2018). “Dr. Piia Varis on conspiracy theories and digital culture” (video). Source: Youtube @diggitmagazine. May 21, 2018.<\/p>\n\n\n\n

Watson, P.J.  (2020) \u201cBat Soup\u201d . Youtube<\/em>, 24 Jan.<\/p>\n\n\n\n

Watson, P.J.  (2020) \u201cCoronavirus\u201d . Youtube<\/em>, 11 March.<\/p>\n\n\n\n

***<\/p>\n\n\n\n[1] It has to be noted that digital tech and social media companies like Google, Facebook, Twitter, Linkedin, YouTube and Reddit have now taken precautions against the spread of misinformation (see Maly 2020). <\/p>\n","protected":false},"excerpt":{"rendered":"

When it comes to the COVID-19 pandemic that has the world in its grip, gaps in public knowledge still abound. It is therefore not surprising that during the last weeks a whole bunch of \u2018interesting\u2019 theories and speculations regarding \u2018what is really going on\u2019 have surfaced. Did the virus originate with very hungry Chinese people […]<\/p>\n","protected":false},"author":1923,"featured_media":12604,"comment_status":"open","ping_status":"closed","sticky":false,"template":"","format":"standard","meta":{"footnotes":""},"categories":[292],"tags":[],"acf":[],"yoast_head":"\nDo COVID-19 conspiracy theories stem from gullibility or skepticism? - International Cognition and Culture Institute<\/title>\n<meta name=\"robots\" content=\"index, follow, max-snippet:-1, max-image-preview:large, max-video-preview:-1\" \/>\n<link rel=\"canonical\" href=\"http:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/blogs\/inge-van-de-ven\/do-covid-19-conspiracy-theories-stem-from-gullibility-or-skepticism\/\" \/>\n<meta property=\"og:locale\" content=\"en_GB\" \/>\n<meta property=\"og:type\" content=\"article\" \/>\n<meta property=\"og:title\" content=\"Do COVID-19 conspiracy theories stem from gullibility or skepticism? - International Cognition and Culture Institute\" \/>\n<meta property=\"og:description\" content=\"When it comes to the COVID-19 pandemic that has the world in its grip, gaps in public knowledge still abound. It is therefore not surprising that during the last weeks a whole bunch of \u2018interesting\u2019 theories and speculations regarding \u2018what is really going on\u2019 have surfaced. Did the virus originate with very hungry Chinese people […]\" \/>\n<meta property=\"og:url\" content=\"http:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/blogs\/inge-van-de-ven\/do-covid-19-conspiracy-theories-stem-from-gullibility-or-skepticism\/\" \/>\n<meta property=\"og:site_name\" content=\"International Cognition and Culture Institute\" \/>\n<meta property=\"article:published_time\" content=\"2020-03-31T07:57:00+00:00\" \/>\n<meta property=\"article:modified_time\" content=\"2023-08-09T12:15:42+00:00\" \/>\n<meta property=\"og:image\" content=\"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/wp-content\/uploads\/media.jpg\" \/>\n\t<meta property=\"og:image:width\" content=\"1640\" \/>\n\t<meta property=\"og:image:height\" content=\"863\" \/>\n<meta name=\"twitter:card\" content=\"summary\" \/>\n<meta name=\"twitter:label1\" content=\"Written by\">\n\t<meta name=\"twitter:data1\" content=\"Inge van de Ven\">\n\t<meta name=\"twitter:label2\" content=\"Estimated reading time\">\n\t<meta name=\"twitter:data2\" content=\"9 minutes\">\n<script type=\"application\/ld+json\" class=\"yoast-schema-graph\">{\"@context\":\"https:\/\/schema.org\",\"@graph\":[{\"@type\":\"WebSite\",\"@id\":\"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/#website\",\"url\":\"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/\",\"name\":\"International Cognition and Culture Institute\",\"description\":\"\",\"potentialAction\":[{\"@type\":\"SearchAction\",\"target\":\"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/?s={search_term_string}\",\"query-input\":\"required name=search_term_string\"}],\"inLanguage\":\"en-GB\"},{\"@type\":\"ImageObject\",\"@id\":\"http:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/blogs\/inge-van-de-ven\/do-covid-19-conspiracy-theories-stem-from-gullibility-or-skepticism\/#primaryimage\",\"inLanguage\":\"en-GB\",\"url\":\"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/wp-content\/uploads\/media.jpg\",\"width\":1640,\"height\":863},{\"@type\":\"WebPage\",\"@id\":\"http:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/blogs\/inge-van-de-ven\/do-covid-19-conspiracy-theories-stem-from-gullibility-or-skepticism\/#webpage\",\"url\":\"http:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/blogs\/inge-van-de-ven\/do-covid-19-conspiracy-theories-stem-from-gullibility-or-skepticism\/\",\"name\":\"Do COVID-19 conspiracy theories stem from gullibility or skepticism? - International Cognition and Culture Institute\",\"isPartOf\":{\"@id\":\"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/#website\"},\"primaryImageOfPage\":{\"@id\":\"http:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/blogs\/inge-van-de-ven\/do-covid-19-conspiracy-theories-stem-from-gullibility-or-skepticism\/#primaryimage\"},\"datePublished\":\"2020-03-31T07:57:00+00:00\",\"dateModified\":\"2023-08-09T12:15:42+00:00\",\"author\":{\"@id\":\"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/#\/schema\/person\/2251601d14daaa229a250f12ab956045\"},\"inLanguage\":\"en-GB\",\"potentialAction\":[{\"@type\":\"ReadAction\",\"target\":[\"http:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/blogs\/inge-van-de-ven\/do-covid-19-conspiracy-theories-stem-from-gullibility-or-skepticism\/\"]}]},{\"@type\":\"Person\",\"@id\":\"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/#\/schema\/person\/2251601d14daaa229a250f12ab956045\",\"name\":\"Inge van de Ven\",\"image\":{\"@type\":\"ImageObject\",\"@id\":\"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/#personlogo\",\"inLanguage\":\"en-GB\",\"url\":\"http:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/wp-content\/uploads\/ingeaugustus-96x96.jpg\",\"caption\":\"Inge van de Ven\"}}]}<\/script>\n<!-- \/ Yoast SEO plugin. -->","_links":{"self":[{"href":"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/wp-json\/wp\/v2\/posts\/12589"}],"collection":[{"href":"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/wp-json\/wp\/v2\/posts"}],"about":[{"href":"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/wp-json\/wp\/v2\/types\/post"}],"author":[{"embeddable":true,"href":"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/wp-json\/wp\/v2\/users\/1923"}],"replies":[{"embeddable":true,"href":"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/wp-json\/wp\/v2\/comments?post=12589"}],"version-history":[{"count":23,"href":"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/wp-json\/wp\/v2\/posts\/12589\/revisions"}],"predecessor-version":[{"id":17961,"href":"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/wp-json\/wp\/v2\/posts\/12589\/revisions\/17961"}],"wp:featuredmedia":[{"embeddable":true,"href":"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/wp-json\/wp\/v2\/media\/12604"}],"wp:attachment":[{"href":"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/wp-json\/wp\/v2\/media?parent=12589"}],"wp:term":[{"taxonomy":"category","embeddable":true,"href":"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/wp-json\/wp\/v2\/categories?post=12589"},{"taxonomy":"post_tag","embeddable":true,"href":"https:\/\/cognitionandculture.local\/wp-json\/wp\/v2\/tags?post=12589"}],"curies":[{"name":"wp","href":"https:\/\/api.w.org\/{rel}","templated":true}]}}